StudentShare
Contact Us
Sign In / Sign Up for FREE
Search
Go to advanced search...
Free

The Aspects of Positive Psychology - Coursework Example

Cite this document
Summary
"The Aspects of Positive Psychology" paper seeks to understand and explain positive emotions as they relate to positive psychology. The goal of positive psychology is to establish an understanding of the elements which ensure that individuals and communities would flourish. …
Download full paper File format: .doc, available for editing
GRAB THE BEST PAPER98.9% of users find it useful
The Aspects of Positive Psychology
Read Text Preview

Extract of sample "The Aspects of Positive Psychology"

Positive Psychology Introduction Positive psychology is considered to be a branch of psychology described by Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000) as a field where favorable human functions are present and which seeks to secure a scientific conceptualization in ensuring that individuals, families, and communities would thrive. Psychologists in this field seek the development of genius and talent and ensuring that normal life is very much fulfilling (Compton, 2005). It is therefore mostly focused on applying psychological theory and interventions to understand the creative and positive elements of human behavior (Seligman, 1998). This paper shall now seek to understand and explain positive emotions as they as they relate to positive psychology. Body The goal of positive psychology is to establish an understanding of the elements which ensure that individuals and communities would flourish. In general, positive emotions seem to provide markers indicating optimal well-being (Fredrickson, 2001). Incidents evoking joy, love, and contentment are usually moments when negative feelings are absent. In effect, the general balance of positive and negative feelings has been declared to be influenced by people’s subjective conditions (Diener, et.al., 1991). Based on such balance, analysts like Kahneman (1999) expressed that objective happiness can appropriately be evaluated by assessing people’s experiences, as well as their good and bad feelings. Based on such viewpoints, positive emotions indicate a flourishing state. Moreover, positive emotions also help a person flourish. They flourish within current pleasant moment, but even beyond such moments as well. The general conclusion which can be drawn therefore is that positive emotions are worth developing, not only as end goals, but also as a tool in ensuring psychological growth and a well-developed personality and state of mind over time. Definitions relating to emotions are very much different. However a general consensus has been established on emotions being a smaller categorization of the bigger class of affective psychology (Fredrickson, 2001). Based on such perspective, emotions can be understood best as a multi-organizational response that manifest over short periods of time. In general, emotions manifest when there is a specific evaluation of the personal meaning of some previous incident (Fredrickson, 2001). Such assessment may sometimes be conscious or unconscious. This leads to various responses which are seen alongside component elements, including subjective experience, cognitive processes, and biological changes. Positive affect as discussed by various theorists helps ensure approach behavior as well as sustained action (Cacioppo, et.al., 1999). Accordingly, positive affect ensures that individuals interact with their environment and participate in various activities, some of which may require adjustments from the individual. The relationship between positive affect and activity engagement helps explain the positivity offset, where people often tend to experience mild positive affect, even under neutral conditions (Ito and Cacioppo, 1999). Where there is no offset, individuals would likely not engage with their surroundings and with other people. However, even with the offset, individuals often manifest an adaptive disparity to new objects and situations (Watson, et.al., 1999). Since positive emotions include an element of positive affect, they also manifest as internal cues to approach (Anderson, et.al., 2003). Regardless, positive emotions manifest such functions along with various positive states. For instance, sensory pleasure prompts individuals to persistently approach any stimulus which is relevant to them at that time (Anderson, et.al., 2003). Positive moods also allow people to carry out their activities towards any direction or action they may have initiated (Anderson, et.al., 2003). In effect, specific accounts on positive emotions which indicate tendencies to approach may only cover the least common element over all affective states which have a pleasant and subjective understanding, often allowing other functions within specific positive emotions to stay uncharted. Theorists discussing discrete emotions correlate specific emotions to the principle of specific action tendencies (Oatley and Jenkins, 1996). As such, fear is sometimes related to escape, anger and the need to attack, as well as disgust and the need to expel. Individuals usually express these urges when they feel specific emotions; and mostly their concept on possible actions focus on various behavioral choices (Oatley and Jenkins, 1996). A main concept from such perspective is that a definite action tendency can help emotions evolve and adapt with time. These tendencies worked well in assisting humans in managing and surviving dangerous situations (Barrett, 2006). Specific action tendencies and biological changes also unfold simultaneously. In effect, when individuals feel the urge to escape fearsome situations, their bodies often trigger autonomic responses, often allowing them to physically react to their fears. Even as specific action tendencies have been cited to indicate applications of positive emotions, the action tendencies for positive emotions have been considered vague (Bonanno, et.al., 2007). Such tendencies are considered too general to be evaluated as specific. They appear to be general urges relating to specific actions like fleeing or attacking. What is problematic is if the action tendencies caused by positive emotions are unclear, their impact on survival may be inconsequential (Bonanno, et.al., 2007). So, as the perspective focusing on generic approach tendencies, the view on specific action tendencies presents an unclear assessment of functions relating to positive emotions. There are various theories which seek to explain positive emotions as they relate to positive psychology. One of these theories is the Broaden and Build theory by Fredrickson. This theory basically indicates that some discrete positive emotions like joy, pride, and interest, though they may be distinct, all have the ability to expand individual thought action repertoires, from biological and intellectual tools to social and psychological resources (Fredrickson, 2001). Although positive emotions can manifest in unfavorable circumstances, the overall conditions for positive emotions is not really a life-threatening condition. In effect, psychological concepts which limit individuals and their actions to support quick actions may not necessarily be needed (Fredrickson, 2001). Instead, feelings of joy, pride, and love seem to have complementary effects as they expand the thought-action choices of people; they also expand the possible actions which come to mind. Conceptual assessment of positive emotions recognizes such claims. Feelings like joy increase as they further improve the urge to play and to be creative (Fredrickson, 2001). These instincts are seen not just in one’s social and physical activities, but also in their intellectual behavior. In other cases, feelings of commitment often triggers the desire to enjoy one’s current life conditions and use such feelings to develop better perceptions of one’s self and the world in general (Boone and Buck, 2003). Love encompasses other positive emotions and is often felt within safe relationships. Love expands one’s outlook as it secures sustained urges of play and of enjoying experiences with loved ones. Such thought-action tendencies including playing and exploring indicate different ways by which positive emotions expand one’s habitual thoughts and actions (Fredrickson, 2000). In relation to negative emotions which have direct adaptive effects on incidents that threaten life, the broadened thought-action choices prompted by positive emotions are favorable in different ways (Fredrickson, 2001). Specifically, such perceptions have indirect and long-term effects; broadening can support personal resources which act as reserves to be used later in protecting against future threats. Play for example is associated with joy. Based on various researches, forms of chasing play have been seen in animals, juveniles of specific species (Dolhinow, 1987). These play activities include climbing branches and catapulting into other branches. Among adult animals of same species, same instincts to run or to flee are seen when they are faced with predators (Dolhinow, 1987). Such interactions indicate that juvenile play can eventually help develop one’s physical qualities. Play also helps support social resources, including social play, excitement, shared amusement, and eventually social attachments (Aron, et.al., 2000). Childhood play therefore helps develop intellectual tools as it also improves creativity; and it also helps improve brain development (Fredrickson and Branigan, 2001). It is crucial to recognize that personal tools gained during instances of positive emotions are considered durable. They last beyond temporary feelings and emotional states which caused their acquisition (Fredrickson and Branigan, 2001). As a result, the incidental impact of the positive emotions implies the increase of personal resources. Such resources are considered reserves which can be used when other emotional incidents are apparent. The broaden-and-build theory therefore supports positive emotions in relation to broadened thought action options and it also supports functions in relation to sustained personal resources (Butler, et.al., 2003). This theory presents another perspective on the relevance of positive emotions. Human ancestors who gave in to the desires triggered by positive emotions would have generally gained more personal resources. As these ancestors were later confronted with threats to their life, their significant personal resources allowed them to increase their chances of survival (Fredrickson, 2001). In effect, to a certain extent, based on the ability to enjoy positive emotions, such ability would have become a necessary part of human nature. Observed support for various principles of the broaden-and-build theory can be based on numerous sub-disciplines in psychology, covering cognition and intrinsic motivation, as well as attachment resources. Evidence indicates that positive emotions expand the coverage of attention and action. They also improve physical and social resources. Based on the work of Isen and colleagues (2000), positive emotions expand individuals’ temporary thought-action choices. Individuals going through positive emotions manifest thought patterns which seem to be unusual, flexible, creative, and efficient (Isen, et.al., 2000). The authors also discovered that individuals going through positive emotions manifest a higher preference for variety; and they also favor a wider range of behavioral options. In effect, authors Isen, et.al., (2000) point out that positive emotions manifests a broad and flexible capacity to integrate diverse materials. Such effects were later associated with increased brain dopamine levels. Isen’s work feature strong evidence on positive emotions and how they improve and broaden cognition. Negative emotions have mostly limited the attention of people; and on the other hand, positive emotions seem to expand perceptions (Derryberry and Tucker, 1994). In fact, negative conditions including anxiety, depression and feelings of failure seem to have a stronger correlation with narrowed attention; whereas, positive emotions, including success and optimism seem to relate to wider and more diverse perceptions (Basso, et.al., 1994). Positive emotions also have a favorable effect as it helps undo lingering negative emotions. Where negative feelings limit the thought-action options and positive feelings expand such options, then positive feelings should serve as effective tools in eliminating the residual impact of negative feelings. In effect, positive emotions would help eliminate the after effects of negative emotions (Fredrickson, 2001). The main observation of positive emotions being incompatible with negative emotions is not a novel observation; this observation has been seen in previous studies covering anxiety disorders (Wolpe, 1958), as well as studies on aggression, Baron, 1976). Still, the exact resource effectively accountable for the incompatibility has not been sufficiently identified. The expanding functions relating to positive emotions may have a bigger role. As there is a broadening of individual’s temporary thought-action choices, positive feelings may expand the impact of negative emotions on individuals, their mind and bodies, by affecting the preparation of specific actions. Positive feelings also seem to trigger psychological resilience. The discussion above indicates that individuals would be able to improve their psychological state and their physical health as they would improve how they would experience positive emotions during specific times in order to manage negative feelings (Fredrickson, 2000). Similar claims on experiences affecting chronic stress help individuals manage their negative emotions (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000). Studies supporting such claims can be taken from experiments indicating the positive emotions ensure attention allocated to negative and self-relevant data (Aspinwall, 1998). These results indicate how positive emotions and beliefs become resources for individuals to manage challenges and adversity (Aspinwall, 1998). It would seem clear that some individuals would likely comprehend and use positive emotions to assist them in their activities. This can be seen in terms of psychological resilience. Resilient individuals often recover quickly from stressful incidents. The analogy is strong in terms of resilient metals bending, but not breaking (Carver, 1998). The theoretical understanding of resilience indicates that in relation to less resilient individuals, the resilient often manifest higher cardiovascular recovery triggered by negative feelings. Moreover, the broaden-and-build theory indicates that the power to restore cardiovascular baselines may be triggered by positive feelings experienced. Positive emotions also help improve psychological resilience and prompt upward spirals of enhanced emotional wellness (Cacioppo, et.al., 2006). Initial signs indicate that positive emotions may help ensure individual differences in resilience. Since psychological resilience is a persistent personal tool, the broaden-and-build theory supports the prediction that positive feelings may eventually improve psychological resilience. In effect, to the point where positive feelings improve the attention and cognition, allowing for creative thinking, they must also enhance and improve people’s coping capacity (Aspinwall, 1998). In turn, as psychological strength is improved, positive feelings can also improve the emotional well-being of individuals. In line with this perspective, studies indicate that individuals who go through positive emotions during times of bereavement would likely go establish long-term plans (Affleck and Tennen, 1996). Alongside positive emotions, plans also lead to improved psychological status following deaths or periods of mourning. People often experience positive feelings during adversity when they also seek the positive meanings of the things going on in their lives (Affleck and Tennen, 1996). It is important to understand that the link between positive meaning as well as positive feelings is reciprocal. This means that finding positive meaning can trigger positive feelings, especially as they expand one’s thinking and improve one’s ability to perceive positive meaning in other events (Fredrickson, 2000). The possible reciprocal associations observed in positive emotions and positive meanings imply that eventually the impact of positive feelings may compound and evolve (Fredrickson, 2001). The wider focus and cognition prompted by previous experiences would likely assist in managing challenges, and improved coping may support other experiences relating to positive emotion. Throughout this cycle, individuals help improve their psychological resilience as well as improve their emotional state. Studies on depression have indicated a negative spiral where depression and pessimistic attitudes influence each other, eventually causing worsening moods and possible clinical depression. The broaden-and-build theory supports an upward spiral where positive emotions and the wider perspectives they trigger also influence each other, causing an increase in emotional conditions (Campbell-Sills, et.al., 2006). Positive feelings may prompt these upward spirals by improving resilience and improving how individuals manage adversity In testing the hypothesis of cognitive broadening leading to an upward spiral and enhances emotional state, Fredrickson (2000) evaluated positive and negative feelings, including broad-minded coping. Broad-minded coping was affected by matters like: thinking of various ways to manage the problem and stepping back from the situation and being more objective. Data indicated clear signs of upward spiral. Individuals who manifested positive emotions were more resilient to challenges. Moreover, their improved coping skills eventually led to more positive feelings. These results indicate that positive emotions as well as broad-minded coping both support each other. Positive feelings help people feel good about their current conditions; and also with favorable effects on broadened thinking, positive feelings increase the possibility of individuals eventually feeling optimistic about their future (Fredrickson, 2001). As broad-minded coping is a type of psychological resilience, data may not match prediction, founded on the broaden-and-build theory that transient experiences of positive emotion can support and increase enduring psychological resources and lead to upward spirals toward better emotional states (Fredrickson, 2001). Much evidence has been gathered supporting the fact that positive emotions are linked with favorable physical and psychological health conditions. Some of these health outcomes have been previously touched on. Specifically, a discussion on these outcomes shall be established in this paper. One positive outcome is laughter and humor. Laughter, as has been mentioned all too often, is a good medicine. It prompts and increases positive feelings (Bachorowski and Owren, 2001). It also leads to feelings of improvement in the immune system functions (Mahony, et.al., 2002). Moreover, based on self-reports on immune functions, laughter is affected by subjective experiences relating to positive emotion, mostly for older individuals (Mahony, et.al., 2002). This implies that positive feelings caused by laughter are significant in predicting improved health goals for the immune system. Positive emotions affect the impact of coping with humor and immune system functions (Dillon, et.al., 1985). Laughter is often related to humor. People may be different in terms of the extent to which they apply humor to manage stressful incidents. Those who are more likely to use humor to cope with problems are more likely to be in a positive mood. In effect, to respond to stress, those who are likely to cope using humor manifest improved salivary immunoglobulin A, which is a crucial immune system protein and is the body’s first line of defense against respiratory diseases (Dillon, et.al., 1985). Supporting studies in an experience-sampling research indicates that self-reported positive feelings revealed increases in IgA levels, implying improved immune functions. These results indicate that individuals who can have positive emotional states throughout stressful situations can manifest improvements in their immune functions (Masten, 2001). Another health condition affected by positive emotions is heart disease and related affectations. For instance, for elderly individuals suffering from cardiovascular diseases reporting greater feelings of happiness for 90 days following hospital release, they also experienced lower hospital admission rates (Tugade, et.al., 2004). Reports expressing positive emotions also indicate readmission which exceeded other elements including health status as well as length and initial stay in the hospital. Cardiovascular health benefits related to positive emotions are further indicated by studies on optimism, a quality supported by sustained positive feelings (Carver and Scheier, 2002). Optimists seem to manifest better rates of physical recovery after coronary artery bypass surgeries. In studies involving war veterans, optimists as compared to the pessimists were also less prove to angina and heart attacks (Kubzansky, et.al., 2001). These soldiers also indicated improved levels of pulmonary functions, decreased rates of pulmonary deterioration, and a protective impact which is not affected by smoking (Kubzansky, et.al., 2002). Consequently, optimism is related to a decreased possibility of disease and improved chances for recovery, mostly due to chronic positive emotional conditions which optimists manifest. Current theories however note that various health impacts relating to dispositional optimism are akin to the effects seen in other positive emotions (Aspinwall and Leaf, 2002). As such, theorists are beginning to understand whether positive feelings, including hope may also improve health benefits experienced by optimists. Positive emotional disclosure is also impacted by positive emotions. Interventions which support positive emotions are helpful to one’s health (Emmons and McCullough, 2003). In one study, participants were grouped into three: count your blessings, list daily hassles and control. Participants who counted their blessings for 10 weeks manifested better health outcomes; they had less physical ails; and were likely to exercise more and sleep better (Emmons and McCullough, 2003). In other studies, several researches indicated that emotional disclosure can lead to improved health functions, especially where positive emotional content is seen in the disclosures (Tugade, et.la., 2004). For example, the application of more positive emotions in describing mild stressors led to less illness-related doctor visits in the span of two months was seen in patients manifesting positive emotional disclosures. Individuals who were also told to write their perceived benefits relating to trauma were likely to experience better health outcomes, including lesser health center visits (King and Miner, 2000). Applying more positive emotion words seemed to benefit individuals going through trauma and other stressful conditions. In the study by Stein and colleagues (1997), caregivers who had patients or partners recently succumbing to AIDS expressed their feelings and experiences 2 weeks, 1 month and 1 year following the death. Those who used more positive-emotion words manifested better positive morale and were less likely to be depressed (Stein, et.al., 1997). All in all, these studies indicate how writing positive emotions can impact favorably on one’s health. Studies also indicate advantages relating to positive emotional disclosure leading to short-term health benefits, with benefits lasting for a lifetime (Danner, et.al., 2001). For instance, positive emotional writings seen in the autobiographies of nuns during their early adulthood predicted the possibility of still being alive 60 years after (Danner, et.al., 2001). In effect, positive emotions seen among elderly adults help contradict physical debility in old age (Ostir, et.al., 2001). Finally, studies on HIV1 males indicate that positive emotions seem to predict a lesser risk of death from AIDS (Moskowitz, 2003). In general, these results support positive emotions being valuable resources in addressing immediate health issues, and also in ensuring long-term beneficial results. Along with the promotion of physical health, managing positive emotions also relates with psychological health (Fredrickson, 2000). For example, coping skills related to the sustenance of positive emotions help afford protection against stress and depression (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000). Such strategies assist individuals in managing crises using new coping skills, gaining closer relations, and ensuring a health appreciation of life. All of these elements translate to a better psychological condition. Empirical support for the manifestation of positive emotions ensures adaptive coping and adjustments relating to acute and chronic stress (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2000). For example, individuals who are able to find meaning while caring for sick partners seemed to cope better with the distress and emotional impact of caregiving and bereavement. Moreover, women who also saw benefits even with hazardous child delivery and prolonged hospitalization following delivery manifested improved well-being, which then led to the development of well-adjusted children (Affleck and Tennen, 1996). The manifestation of positive feelings alongside challenges may secure the much needed psychological respite against stress, allowing for the restoration of coping abilities. Positive emotions are related to improved psychological functioning, including improved levels of well-being and lesser possibilities of health issues (Lyubomirsky, et.al., 2005). These links may seem obvious because individuals who have positive emotions would likely have better health. Moreover, positive emotions form part of the delineation of elements relating to psychological functioning. For instance, wellness is usually related to high levels of positive emotion (Diener, 2000). What seems surprising is the fact the positive emotions seem to affect psychological functions. This is seen in various experimental studies (Lyubomirsky, et.al., 2005). Fredrickson and colleagues (2008) assigned participants to meditation practice in order to improve positive emotions; other participants were considered control groups. With time, the group in the positive emotion conditions manifested improved emotions which also led to improved well-being and reduced depressed symptoms. These elements were affected by gains in terms of health and social benefits. Results indicating that positive emotions lead to improved outcomes also led to questions on how positive emotions may do so. Based on discussions by Keltner and Haidt (1999), a crucial function of emotions is to manage an individual’s social relations and his relationships. Specifically, positive emotions support these functions by relating to others their intent and approachability by triggering positive emotional conditions in other individuals (King, 2000). In effect, positive feelings support social connections and the establishment of long-term bonds (Anderson, et.al., 2003). The main suggestion here is that positive emotions would likely have adaptive social results to the extent that they are then transmitted well to others. Where positive feelings are not expressed well, they would likely lose some of their positive results. In effect, to understand how positive emotions are adaptive, there is a need to focus the issue on whether they are communicated well. It is important to indicate that accuracy is achieved and communication becomes most effective where positive emotions are reflected in one’s feelings. Conclusion The question being asked mostly relates to the role which positive emotions have in positive psychology. Past perspectives indicate that positive feelings indicate wellness and often guide current behavior. Without downplaying the relevance of these functions, the broaden-and-build theory pushes positive feelings into a more relevant spotlight. This theory indicates that positive emotions, although temporary also have a more sustained impact. Based on the broaden-and-build theory, positive feelings are tools which can assist in individual improvement and in building social connections. They improve the personal and the social resources of the individual; these positive emotions change people for the better, allowing them to eventually gain better lives. The broaden-and-build theory also indicates the various discrete and positive feelings are needed to ensure optimal functions. In effect, the capacity to go through various emotions might be interpreted as basic human strengths which help produce significant benefits. References Affleck, G. and Tennen, H. (1996). Construing benefits from adversity: Adaptational significance and dispositional underpinnings. Journal of Personality, 64, pp. 899–922. Anderson, C., Keltner, D. and John, O. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, pp. 1054–1068. Aron, A., Norman, C., Aron, E., McKenna, C. and Heyman, R. (2000). Couples shared participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, pp. 273–284. Aspinwall, L. (1998). Rethinking the role of positive affect in self-regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 22, pp. 1–32. Aspinwall, L. and Leaf, S. (2002). In search of the unique aspects of hope: Pinning our hopes on positive emotions, future oriented thinking hard times, and other people. Psychological Inquiry, 13, pp. 276–288. Bachorowski, J. and Owren, M. (2001). Not all laughs are alike: Voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psychological Science, 12, pp. 252–257. Baron, R. (1976). The reduction of human aggression: A field study of the influence of incompatible reactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 6, pp. 260–274. Barrett, L. (2006). Emotions as natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, pp. 28–58. Bonanno, G., Colak, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M., Papa, A., and Noll, J. (2007). Context matters: The benefits and costs of expressing positive emotion among survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Emotion, 7, pp. 824–837. Boone, T. and Buck, R. (2003). Emotional expressivity and trustworthiness: The role of nonverbal behavior in the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, pp. 163–182. Butler, E., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F., Smith, N., Erickson, E., and Gross, J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3, pp. 48–67. Cacioppo, J., Gardner, W., and Berntson, G. (1999). The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, pp. 839–855. Cacioppo, J., Hughes, M., Waite, L., Hawkley, L. and Thisted, R. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms. Psychology and Aging, 21, pp. 140 –151. Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D., Brown, T. and Hofmann, S. (2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses on individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, pp. 1251–1263. Carver, C. (1998). Resilience and thriving: Issues, models, and link-ages. Journal of Social Issues, 54, pp. 245–266. Carver, C. and Scheier, M. (2002). Optimism. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.). Handbook of positive psychology. London: Oxford University Press. Compton, W. (2005). An introduction to positive psychology. New York: Wadsworth Publishing. Danner, D., Snowdon, D. and Friesen, W. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80, pp. 804–813. Derryberry, D. and Tucker, D. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In: Neidenthal PM, Kitayama S, editors. The hearts eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention. California: Academic Press Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American Psychologist, 55, pp. 34–43. Diener, E., Sandvik, E. and Pavot, W. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not the intensity, of positive versus negative affect. In: Strack F, editor. Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Dillon, K., Minchoff, B. and Baker, K. (1985). Positive emotional states and enhancement of the immune system. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 15, pp. 13–18. Dolhinow, P. (1987). At play in the fields. In: Topoff H, editor. The natural history reader in animal behavior. New York: Columbia University Press. Folkman, S. and Moskowitz, J. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. American Psychologist, 55, pp. 647–654. Emmons, R. and McCullough, M. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 84, pp. 377–389. Fredrickson, B. (2000). Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and well-being. Prevention and Treatment [online]. Available at: http://www.journals.apa.org/prevention/volume3/pre0030001a.html. [Accessed 10 June 2013]. Fredrickson, B. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. Am Psychol, 56(3), pp. 218–226. Fredrickson, B. and Branigan, C. (2001). Positive emotions. In: Mayne TJ, Bonnano GA, editors. Emotion: Current issues and future directions. New York: Guilford Press. Fredrickson, B., Cohn, M., Coffey, K., Pek, J. and Finkel, S. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, pp. 1045–1062. Isen, A. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, editors. Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press. Ito, T. and Cacioppo, J. (1999). The psychophysiology of utility appraisals. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwartz N, editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Keltner, D. and Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 13, pp. 505–521. King, L. (2000). Why happiness is good for you: A commentary on Fredrickson. Prevention & Treatment, 3, Article 4. King, L. and Miner, K. (2000). Writing about the perceived benefits of traumatic events: Implications for physical health. Personality and social psychology bulletin, 26, pp. 220–230. Kubzansky, L., Wright, R., Cohen, S., Weiss, S., and Rosner, B., (2002). Breathing easy: A prospective study of optimism and pulmonary function in the normative aging study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 24, pp. 345–353. Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. and Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, pp. 803–855. Mahony, D., Burroughs, W., and Lippman, L. (2002). Perceived attributes of health-promoting laughter: A cross-generational comparison. Journal of Psychology, 136, pp. 171–181. Masten, A. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56, pp. 227–238. Moskowitz, J. (2003). Positive affect predicts lower risk of AIDS mortality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, pp. 620–626. Oatley, K. and Jenkins, J. (1996). Understanding emotions. Massachusetts: Blackwell Ostir, G., Markides, K., Peek, M., and Goodwin, J. (2001). The association between emotional well-being and the incidence of stroke in older adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, pp. 210–215. Seligman, M. (1998). Learned optimism. New York: Pocket Books. Seligman, M. and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55 (1), pp. 5–14. Stein, N., Folkman, S., Trabasso, T. and Richards, A. (1997). Appraisal and goal processes as predictors of psychological well-being in bereaved caregivers. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72, pp. 872–884. Tugade, M., Fredrickson, B., and Barrett, L. (2004). Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. Journal of Personality 72(6). Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J. and Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, pp. 820–838. Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. California: Stanford University Press. Read More
Cite this document
  • APA
  • MLA
  • CHICAGO
(The Aspects of Positive Psychology Coursework Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 4000 words, n.d.)
The Aspects of Positive Psychology Coursework Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 4000 words. https://studentshare.org/psychology/1802081-write-a-paper-on-one-aspect-of-positive-psychology
(The Aspects of Positive Psychology Coursework Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 4000 Words)
The Aspects of Positive Psychology Coursework Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 4000 Words. https://studentshare.org/psychology/1802081-write-a-paper-on-one-aspect-of-positive-psychology.
“The Aspects of Positive Psychology Coursework Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 4000 Words”. https://studentshare.org/psychology/1802081-write-a-paper-on-one-aspect-of-positive-psychology.
  • Cited: 0 times

CHECK THESE SAMPLES OF The Aspects of Positive Psychology

William James : Habit (From The Principles of Psychology)

Within this book he considers a number of different aspects of psychology that play a large role in humans, including habit.... psychology is one of the most essential areas in the sphere of science and human life and is studied in an attempt to understand why people behave in the manner in which they do and to comprehend their subconscious (Benjafield 1-4).... He has written a number of texts on the subject including The Principles of psychology in 1980....
4 Pages (1000 words) Research Paper

Creating a Plan for Positive Influence

In organizations comprise diversified people who represent different cultures with varied political and religious perspectives.... Due to these facts, the employees of an organization bring their varied emotions, attitudes, core values and personalities to their workplaces.... … All these elements usually shape the behaviors of these employees in the work place therefore there is a need to make them aware of the impacts that these elements impact on the organization as whole (Topchik, 2001)....
4 Pages (1000 words) Essay

Sex and Drugs in Psychology

In the end, however, there is risk associated with most aspects of life.... The use of drugs in the field of psychology is not a new practice.... hellip; Sex and Drugs in psychology.... The use of drugs in the field of psychology is not a new practice.... What is reality, however, is that there are individuals on both sides of the issue in terms of the advantages and disadvantages of the use of sex and drugs in psychology....
3 Pages (750 words) Essay

Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Personality Questionnaire (NEO-PI)

Extraversion includes positive emotion, Assertiveness, seeking excitement, warmth, growth, activity, Gregariousness.... Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Personality Questionnaire (NEO-PI) Name Institution Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Personality Questionnaire (NEO-PI) The NEO-PI or the NEO personality inventory is a psychological personality theory or inventory that tests or measures the big five personality behaviors or traits....
3 Pages (750 words) Assignment

Outline of Postive Psychology

psychology viewed people as victims of unconscious drives, innate, needs, families and even the environment.... Second advantage is being able to achieve reliable and valid assessment instruments thereby expanding the research into a more comprehensive classification system of humans strengths… Environmental mastery referring to successful adaptation to situations that may involve demand and expectations I....
4 Pages (1000 words) Essay

How did psychology effect our personally

The negative and positive psychology of leadership and group research.... My opinion regarding the psychological settings is that it is comprehensive from all angles and covers many of the aspects that others would like to forego.... It is a fact that psychology is something that puts things into proper equations… psychology has been able to change my thinking because it has made alternate arrangements in the wake of trying circumstances....
2 Pages (500 words) Essay

Psychology in Workplace

Through emotional intelligence, you can manage counterproductive emotions or feelings such as anger and stress and embrace positive feelings such as creativity, optimism, and hope (Aamodt 463).... Apart from profit making which is the primary objective of an organization; they also try to develop a culture for the well-being of the employees....
4 Pages (1000 words) Essay

Adjustment and Happiness in Psychology

the aspects of adjustment are therefore defined in terms of satisfying the needs and requirements of the body.... The paper "Adjustment and Happiness in psychology" describes that psychological adjustment is a factor that influences the experience of happiness among different individuals....
8 Pages (2000 words) Essay
sponsored ads
We use cookies to create the best experience for you. Keep on browsing if you are OK with that, or find out how to manage cookies.
Contact Us