StudentShare
Contact Us
Sign In / Sign Up for FREE
Search
Go to advanced search...
Free

Semantic Interference as an Indication of Competitive Lexical Access - Essay Example

Cite this document
Summary
The paper "Semantic Interference as an Indication of Competitive Lexical Access" states that devising semantic context to influence speech production within various processing levels. Context impacts the conceptual processing based on the semantic priming for target concepts…
Download full paper File format: .doc, available for editing
GRAB THE BEST PAPER93.8% of users find it useful
Semantic Interference as an Indication of Competitive Lexical Access
Read Text Preview

Extract of sample "Semantic Interference as an Indication of Competitive Lexical Access"

Semantic interference is supportive evidence for competitive lexical access in speech production. Discuss the validity of this ment. : Date Semantic interference is an indication of competitive lexical access within speech production. The paper discuses the validity of this statement through lexical entries selected through competitors. However, the concepts have a receipt or activation through taking assumption of co-activated concepts for lexical nodes with stronger influences for lexical interference and conceptual facilitation. The paper illustrates on the support of semantic contexts that yield conceptual priming while varying semantic facilitation effects. Semantic facilitation has an effect on the wide interpretation of contextual and conceptual elements. For example, the context-induced facilitation influences the motivation of message-congruency accounts holding semantic facilitation as obtained through target contexts converging on a given concept. In such account, sustainability of convergence translates into the conceptual facilitation while outweighing the weaker lexical interference impacts. Further, support geared towards the assumption of semantic contexts develops conceptual priming against various studies that reveal the semantic facilitation effects (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). Faster speeds of translation along scheduled words (for example, translating English dog to read in Dutch hond) is superimposed on the semantic relationship of pictures (such as the cat). This compares to unrelated pictures (such as the tree). The interpretation facilitates the effect evidenced by distractor picture engraved in the semantic representation of target words and speeding concepts selection (Trompelt, 2010). Null effects are established while interference becomes one of the expectations taken in facilitation of semantic contexts. The analysis fails to adhere to the anticipated semantic interference influences within picture-picture interference interpretations. The argument is that interference is neutralized through comparable sizing and priming effects from conceptual levels. While network models adopt conceptual-level explanations for the semantic facilitation implications, alternative models attribute such effects towards lexical level processes mediated through conceptual activation (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). By contrast, effects are interference-dominant in case more inter-related competitors, and the lexical cohort are active. Cohort-induced elements to lexical competition outweigh the element of conceptual priming. A related augmentation includes proposal that lexical and semantic activation spreads through highly dynamic, adjustable and flexible contexts where utterances are produced. In the end, activation of the lexical cohorts within word production processes is modulated based on contextual factors (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). The contextual sensitivity allows for lineage with ad hoc notions that categorize formation. Novel categories can be constructed for purposes of achieving goals. For instance, when one of stranded on desert islands, it is possible to quickly construct ad hoc array of activities that can catch rain water (Trompelt, 2010). Theorists suggest that such mechanisms underlying goal-driven formation of ad hoc categories drive flexibilities in observing the lexical and conceptual systems. Even though, the element of lexical cohort activation has, a theoretical inclusion of new concepts, consequences for any form of lexical selection latencies and semantic context effects do not establish gainful interaction. Recent debates on locus of semantic interference show that there are effects that fail to establish factorial considerations. Discussions are based on relative activation levels where the target includes single lexical competition. On the other hand, while the concentration starts with expository purposes, there is a crept concept that theorizes the underlying research (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). It is important to have theoretical accounts for effects of semantic context that retain lexical competition. The critical concern of the account is the assumption that is either implicitly or explicitly incorporated in different speech production models. The initial assumption includes activation of interrelated lexical cohort competitors as a critical determinant of lexical competition while establishing strength of detectable semantic interferences. Activation of single isolated competition is typically insufficient in inducing measurable interference (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). The assumption is solicited from models of implementing the selection mechanisms including the Luce ratio. However, it is consistent with the models of implementing competition through inhibitory links between active representations. The Luce ratio states that the probability to select target lemma in a given time is dependent on state of activation divided by sum activation of other critical lemma nodes. The assumption is latency of the target lemma selection is different from the function of state of activation and entire lexical networks. The proportionality of analysis is delayed due to the increase in number of active competition. The polarity reversals are naturally attached to inherent differences of categorical and associative relations in a lexical cohort account. While distractor and target are placed within a given category (such as a bee and a horse) spreading of the convergent activation to various levels of competition leads to category nodes and other semantic features (such as fly, snake). For this reason, an entire cohort for interrelated competition and lexical nodes has a co-activation and swinging network (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). The convergent activation and interconnectivity of cohort illustrates that most of the activation remains along the cohort while representation becomes a reinforcement of activation of entire sets. This results in the comparative inclusion of high activation levels that produce strong effects of lexical competition effects (Trompelt, 2010). On the contrary, if competitor and target have an associative relationship and belong to distinct semantic categories, (such as bee and honey), activation diverges onto alternative related concepts. This is due to the contemporary issues that do not share common category nodes, as well as the scope of shared semantic features. Activation from concepts target and competitor diverges to representations that are mutually unrelated (such as bread, dessert, and comb). The activated concepts pass their trigger of activation to more unrelated concepts for purposes of activating the dissipated and insubstantial levels. The divergent activation amounts to wide-spread activation where central high activation loci within the origin influence the target and distractor (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). The pattern of activation continues to spread while giving rise to personalized competition within lexical network as compared to one-to-many levels of competition. The blocking paradigm explores specific mechanisms underlying the polarity reversals as illustrated for associative and categorical semantic relations. The experiments and semantic interference are stated for homogeneous blocks that consist of category members (such as bee, horse, and mouse). The implications also apply to homogeneous blocks that consist of associates of different categories belonging to specific semantic contexts (such as honey, bee, and comb) as compared to mainstream heterogeneous blocks. In contrast to the presentation of isolated distractor words, the element of semantic blocking creates situations that harbor lexical entries and related concepts through synergistic activation of each other (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). Blocking paradigm for lexical semantic interference impacts does not restrict category members to the association of different categories due to lexical cohorts activated in each case of relations. Observation in which non-categorical relations induces interference through cohort items interrelating with meaningful ways that support assumptions of competition. The semantic contexts are boosted through the expansion of personal relations as compared to public relations of swinging lexical networks. The swinging network proposal does not introduce a new approach to addressing interference. Lexical cohort activation allows for ingredients from diverse existing speech models of production to interact. However, the results of the levels of competition, as dictated by semantic contexts, receive minimal attention. Swinging network proposals do not necessitate entirety for the qualitative differences of assumptions and effects from different forms of semantic relations (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). This includes assumption that the flow of the levels of activation between lexical and conceptual levels has been restricted. However, the interference covers a broad range of information on semantic context impacts such as the polarity exceptions and reversals of classic interference variances. The elements in question are interpreted as the contra models of lexical competition. Gains from explanatory potential are afforded through swinging network proposals attained in the absence of dismissal and induced lexical competition (Trompelt, 2010). The literature does not influence the essence of attributing underlying mechanisms to the qualitative and different processes. Even though most reviewed context effects have been facilitative above inhibitory, swinging network assumptions accommodate the observations through retention of lexical competition as the main lexical selection element. Critics present alternative frameworks that explain the facilitative and effects inhibitory in picture semantic naming. The proposal is built on the assumptions that have an implicit and largely neglected model. The element re-emphasizes on the significance of lexical cohorts along competition mechanisms and the underlying lexical selection. The application approach recognizes that semantic contexts affect word production at conceptual levels. This is because the priming effects arise at lexical levels where interference intensifies. The model outlines flexible alternatives that conceptual networks recruit context-appropriate lexical cohort concepts (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). The lexical cohorts comprise concepts connected to common contexts, themes, and categories while the activation of the system has sufficient convergence. The procedures suggest that the notion of having flexible swinging network remains crucial in establishing semantic context effects as observed in various experimental paradigms (Crowther, 2009). The selective reviews of critical findings are based on semantic context effects that frameworks provide significant explanatory coverage. Further, results are incompatible with selection and competition affecting the scope of making unique and testable predictions. The cognate advantages account for framework structures of serial architecture. The benefit of cognate pictures includes a reflection on frequency effects on speakers. The phonemes’ combinations constitute of cognates that are frequented through the happening of the occurrence of languages, such as the Catalan–Spanish cognates. In case naming latencies take critical parts in the functioning of frequencies of phoneme combination, it is expected that cognate words are named faster. This is because they are composed, on average, through frequently occurring combinations of phonemes. The account has logically plausible and hypothetical lack of data (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). Alternative observation approaches show that frequencies phoneme combination influences the naming of latencies. Reaction-time for data, as reviewed, goes far in advancing natural accounts for cascading models and serial models that accommodate ad hoc assumptions of experimental paradigms. However, there are various lexical access aspects. If the assumptions hold in advance, serial hypothesis can be saved. The data obtained demands for revision of serial hypothesis. To achieve brevity, the illustrations find the model ideal in appreciating complex dual-task paradigms (Lee, 2009). The two-stage theoretical illustrations have been challenged on various fronts. Connectionist and activation-spreading aspects from lexical access predict that the selected (such as, target) item is phonologically activated. Further, the effects are felt on the activated items through existing semantic alternative. The consequence of mechanics from connectionist networks allows lexicon to advance the conception involving various levels of nodes. The top of a conceptual level where nodes are a representation of concepts and conceptual features that is similar. When achieving the activation, they tend to spread such activation into middle lexical levels. Here, the nodes represent the lexical items and Kempens terminology of lemmas (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). For example, if conceptual nodes that represent animation are active, they spread the activation to animate lexical nodes. The focus is inclusive of bear, sheep and lion. The activated lexical nodes can be equaled to semantic cohort as introduced earlier. The lexical activation pattern results from certain conceptual activation pattern depending on the scope of connections between lexical and conceptual levels. The activated nodes are selected from target items. The activated lexical aspects spread the activation across the bottom and phonological levels (Wagers, 2008). In such case, the nodes were a representation of the aspects of the phonological structure. In particular, the phoneme nodes as well as dependency on theory nodes are aspects of phoneme clusters along the phonological features. Natural property from sketched connectivity patterns between the levels are activated through lexical items and lemma while spreading their activation to constituent phonological level nodes. On the other hand, two-stage theories illustrate that activation-spreading models have a single selected target and a co-activated semantic element of phonologically activity. The phonological instruments are semantically activated through lexical relevance receiving higher activation along the target item units of the eventual selection and articulation. The schematic time-course is a prediction of phonological and semantic encoding in its simplest case. Activation spreads through conceptual lexical levels while semantic activation for target items rises to critical levels (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). In different theories, there are assumptions that target item activation is reduced to periods of resting the level shortly through selection processes. Various things happen even through the simplest case in which the forward activation spreads is semantic activation of resting levels. Phonological activations of targets start after semantic activation while overlapping performance in due course. Phonological activation offers an increment of articulation. A remarkable perspective is the curve for phonological activation along semantic alternatives. The flat curve does not have an impact on the two-stage theorem (Nickels, 2014). The target items, as well as their semantic alternatives, have a simultaneous spreading of their activation within phonological levels. The selection of target items is a phonological activation process that increases the phonological activation for decay in semantic alternatives (Zubicaray, Hansen & McMahon, 2013). The paper concludes that devising semantic context to influence speech production within various processing levels. Context impacts the conceptual processing based on the semantic priming for target concepts. Further, semantic context affects lexical level processes based on the levels of competition between lexical selection entries. The effects develop a collection of inhibitory and facilitative influences. Polarity of the observed effects allows for the naming of latencies and resultant aspects of trade-offs between lexical competition and conceptual facilitation. The opposing effects develop a win of the strong dependence on inter-related lexical elements cohort the sufficient sizes as reactivated. Lexical entries should be selected from competitors while concepts are received from activation policies. The assumption is based on the need to increase the scope of co-activated concepts, as well as lexical nodes with stronger influences on the lexical interferences as compared to conceptual facilitation. References Crowther, J. (2009). Mechanisms and Scope of Planning in Language Production. New York: ProQuest. de Zubicaray, G., Hansen, S., McMahon, K. L. (2013). Differential processing of thematic and categorical conceptual relations in spoken word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Vol 142(1), 131-142. Lee, K. (2009). Phonological and Semantic Processing of Chinese Characters. New York: ProQuest. Nickels, L. (2014). Spoken Word Production and Its Breakdown In Aphasia. New York: Psychology Press. Trompelt, H. (2010). Production of Regular and Non -regular Verbs: Evidence for a Lexical Entry Complexity Account. New York: Universitätsverlag Potsdam. Wagers, M. (2008). The Structure of Memory Meets Memory for Structure in Linguistic Cognition. New York: ProQuest. Read More
Cite this document
  • APA
  • MLA
  • CHICAGO
(Semantic interference is supportive evidence for competitive lexical Essay, n.d.)
Semantic interference is supportive evidence for competitive lexical Essay. https://studentshare.org/education/1859556-semantic-interference-is-supportive-evidence-for-competitive-lexical-access-in-speech-production-discuss-the-validity-of-this-statement
(Semantic Interference Is Supportive Evidence for Competitive Lexical Essay)
Semantic Interference Is Supportive Evidence for Competitive Lexical Essay. https://studentshare.org/education/1859556-semantic-interference-is-supportive-evidence-for-competitive-lexical-access-in-speech-production-discuss-the-validity-of-this-statement.
“Semantic Interference Is Supportive Evidence for Competitive Lexical Essay”. https://studentshare.org/education/1859556-semantic-interference-is-supportive-evidence-for-competitive-lexical-access-in-speech-production-discuss-the-validity-of-this-statement.
  • Cited: 0 times

CHECK THESE SAMPLES OF Semantic Interference as an Indication of Competitive Lexical Access

Hallidayan Approach and Categorisation

However, a lexical system is open because various new additions are constantly taking place in language.... He attaches the real reason behind language to providing meaning in social life and thus argues that all forms of languages are based on three semantic components.... The first semantic component is ideational and works as a resource for interpreting human experience.... The second semantic component is interpersonal and serves to perform various functions (which may be simple or complex) that define social relationships....
6 Pages (1500 words) Essay

Semantic Web and Implications

As the paper, semantic Web and Implications, declares audiences of this concept Research paper are the readers who are tech savvy including computer engineers, students and bloggers.... The semantic Web helps to unify this complicated data in the background, without changing the layout.... According to the discussion semantic Web semantic Web is the extension of current web which is better at enabling the computers and individuals to work in cooperation....
4 Pages (1000 words) Essay

Computer and Technology / Web publishing / Semantic Web

So that any one can access that data quickly and relief work can be started easily.... If there is a condition of deserter at any place access to appropriate information is important for relief actions in crisis circumstances.... The semantic web is the dream of containing data on the web which is defined and connected in a method so that it can be utilized by machines not only for the display reasons, as well as for automation, incorporation and reuse of given data and information transversely in a… semantic web services expand web services and facilities with well-off proper descriptions of their abilities, therefore making ease for automatic detection, composition, active binding, and summons of services inside open surroundings....
2 Pages (500 words) Essay

Ethical Analysis in Medical Indications

Another harm would be expected when the patient gains access to his lab results and knows the diagnosis by himself.... This coursework "Ethical Analysis in Medical Indications" describes the role of ethics in medicine.... This paper comprises reviewing medical situations, clinical discussions, diagnosis as well as identifying treatment options....
6 Pages (1500 words) Coursework

Semantic change

Part b) The present time meaning of the name journey represent a narrowing semantic shift.... However in the year 1935 semantic shift took place with the meaning evolving and was connected with sexuality to mean homosexuality.... However, the strangeness of homosexuality made the semantic shift in meaning, but the word also retained its original meaning....
2 Pages (500 words) Case Study

Interference Colors

The act of light slowing down as it passes through a substance is measured using a number referred to as the interference Colors An interference color is a color that is exhibited by a segment of an anisotropic mineral under cross-polarized light.... interference colors are thus an effect that emanates because different substances have different index of reflection (Ulery, & Drees 185).... interference colors can be understood by utilizing color charts that are contained in most of the course readings....
1 Pages (250 words) Essay

Converseness and Reversions Sense Relations

Within vertical structures, lexical relations are alienated into hyponymy/hyperonyny and meronymy/holonymy.... Within vertical structures, lexical relations are alienated into hyponymy/hyperonyny and meronymy/holonymy.... (1986) lexical Semantics.... Synonymy in particular,… s not further classified in elexiko, but is used to refer to all types of semantic identity, ranging from complete sameness and propositional characteristics to more vague categories such as near-synonymy....
1 Pages (250 words) Essay

How We Forget - the Interference and the Decay Theories

The theory of cognitive psychology assumes that the process of forgetting is characterized by a decrease of the ability to remember things and reproduce the known information; in general, it could be described as the loss of access to the previously remembered data.... The paper "How We Forget - the interference and the Decay Theories" presents debates on whether the interference or the Decay theory to be more accurate.... nbsp;… For the paper, we have chosen the trace decay theory and the interference theory as the most interesting from our point of view....
5 Pages (1250 words) Literature review
sponsored ads
We use cookies to create the best experience for you. Keep on browsing if you are OK with that, or find out how to manage cookies.
Contact Us