StudentShare
Contact Us
Sign In / Sign Up for FREE
Search
Go to advanced search...
Free

Why Is It Difficult to Identify Children Who Are at the Mild End of the Autistic Spectrum - Essay Example

Cite this document
Summary
The paper 'Why Is It Difficult to Identify Children Who Are at the Mild End of the Autistic Spectrum?' shall discuss the reasons for difficulties in recognizing children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum. A child in a state of autism appears to be self-centered since he shows little response to the outside world…
Download full paper File format: .doc, available for editing
GRAB THE BEST PAPER94% of users find it useful
Why Is It Difficult to Identify Children Who Are at the Mild End of the Autistic Spectrum
Read Text Preview

Extract of sample "Why Is It Difficult to Identify Children Who Are at the Mild End of the Autistic Spectrum"

Running Head: WHY IS IT DIFFICULT TO IDENTIFY CHILDREN WHO ARE AT THE Why is it difficult to identify children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum [Writer's Name] [Institution's Name] Why is it difficult to identify children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum Introduction This paper shall discuss why is it difficult to identify children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum. Autism literally means living in terms of the self. To an observer, a child in a state of autism appears to be self-centered since he shows little response to the outside world. However, paradoxically, the child in such a state has little awareness of being a 'self'. In 1960, autism was reported in 4 to 5 cases per 10,000 children. The prevalence of cases rose to 5 to 31 cases per 10,000 children in 1990 (Iovannone, Dunlap, Huber, & Kincaid, 2003). Discussion Autism is usually detected in children, and it has some very unique characteristics which identify an autistic child. Difficulties in areas of communication are the core of autism. Autism is a developmental disorder affecting social interaction, verbal and non-verbal communication skills. (Norbury& Bishop 2002) Autistic children typically do not follow the usual patterns of childhood development and while most cases can be detected before the age of three, not all cases are diagnosed so early. Typically, parents first notice that their child does not interact, babble, talk or respond like other children, sometimes suspecting hearing problems before autism is diagnosed. (Couteur et al. 2005) Detection of children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum is at times a very difficult task. Most autistic children do not make eye contact and have poor attention duration. They are often unable to use gestures, either as a primary of communication, as in sign language, or to accompany verbal communication such as pointing to the object that they want. (Letts & Leinonen, 2001) Some autistic children speak in a high-pitched voice or use robot-like speech. They are often unresponsive to the speech of others and may not respond to their own names. For many, speech and language develop, to some degree, but not to a normal ability level. The development is usually uneven. (Frith 1991) This is a normal condition. There is little awareness of the outside world as such, which is experienced in the mode of the infant's bodily organs, processes and zones. A normal infant emerges from this state because of an innate disposition to recognize patterns, similarities, repetitions and continuities. (Smith et al. 1998) Detection of children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum is difficult, as the raw materials for such mental processes as recognition, classification, object creation and empathy. Through these processes, the child builds an inner representation of commonly agreed reality and becomes self-conscious. The study of children who have failed to emerge satisfactorily from the state of normal primary autism brings home to us the complexity and delicacy of the time-consuming process of becoming aware of the world and its objects, persons and other minds. (Gerlach 1996) When things have gone grossly wrong with these early cognitive processes we say that the child is psychotic. The degree to which he is out of touch with reality distinguishes the psychotic from the neurotic child. However, it is important to make the point that the normal healthy infant who starts life by being out of touch with reality is not psychotic. But he is autistic. (Hollander et al. 2001) Autism is a "spectrum disorder," meaning that it manifests itself differently in each child. However, Detection of children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum is not an easy task. Like a row of dominoes, if an early aspect of development is affected in a particular case of autism, other later-emerging aspects of development will be affected too. As a result, each case of autism presents a slightly different profile of learning abilities and learning disabilities. (Newport, 2002) Each learning ability and each learning disability may influence how a particular child with autism may or may not learn something the way other children without autism may learn that same thing. These autism-specific learning barriers are referred here as autistic learning disabilities. The autism-specific learning strengths are referred to as autistic learning styles. (Gernsbacher et al. 2005) Related to differential diagnosis, the cognitive profiles of children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have been a subject of considerable interest to practitioners and researchers. Both groups have recognized the diagnostic import of identifying unique and distinguishing ASD characteristics. Thus, several researchers have attempted to determine whether the profiles obtained from the Wechsler scales (Wechsler, 2001) could discriminate among Asperger syndrome, autism, and other conditions such as attention disorders, head injury, schizophrenia, and dyslexia (Asarnow, Tanguay, Bott, & Freeman, 2000; Dennis, Lockyer, Lazenby, Donnelly, Wilkinson, & Schoonheyt, 1999; Ehlers et al., 1997; Rumsey & Hamburger, 2003). Lincoln, Courchesne, Kilman, Elmasian, and Allen (2000) found a consistent, uneven pattern in the cognitive profiles of children with high-functioning autism, including a significantly higher Performance IQ when compared to Verbal IQ scores. Children with high-functioning autism obtained their highest scores on Block Design and Object Assembly and their lowest scores on Comprehension and Vocabulary. Lincoln et al. (2000) inferred that the pattern of subtest scale scores they identified was reliable and robust enough to assist in correctly making a diagnosis in children, adolescents, and adults without mental retardation who are suspected of having autism. However, they advised professionals not to exclusively use the pattern of Wechsler subtest scale scores as diagnostic criteria for autism. Ehlers et al. (1997) also interpreted the results of their research to preclude using only a Wechsler profile to make a diagnosis of Asperger syndrome. More than 20 studies to date have reported an unusually uneven Wechsler subtest scale profile among children with ASD of varying ages and ability levels. Various studies have revealed a relatively consistent pattern: strong performance on the Block Design subtest of the Performance scale, and relatively weak performance on the Comprehension subtest of the Verbal scale. The Block Design subtest of the Performance scale is considered a nonverbal concept formation task that requires perceptual organization, spatial visualization, and abstract conceptualization (Sattler, 1999). It is believed to be the best measure of g, Spearman's (2001) general mental energy factor. The Comprehension subtest is designed to assess an children's understanding of social mores and interpersonal situations. Success on this task implies that the children has good social judgment, common sense, and a grasp of social conventionality (Sattler, 1999). Only 4 of the 22 studies specifically included children diagnosed with Asperger syndrome (Bowler, 1992; Dennis et al., 1999; Ehlers et al., 1997; Szatmari, Tuff, Finlayson, & Bartolucci, 2000), and the results of these studies revealed inconsistent psychometric assessment findings. Dennis et al. (1999) and Szatmari et al. (2000) found no obvious differences in IQ subtest scaled scores between children with high-functioning autism and children with Asperger syndrome. However, a statistical comparison could not be made in Dennis et al.'s (1999) study because the number of children was too small (Asperger, n = 4; high-functioning autism, n = 4). Ehlers et al.'s (1997) study reported that children with Asperger syndrome demonstrated good verbal ability and weaknesses on Object Assembly and Coding. The lowest scoring verbal subtest for the children with Asperger syndrome and autism was Arithmetic, not Comprehension, as was characteristic of almost all of the other 21 studies. As an explanation for these findings, Ehlers et al. (1997) indicated that many of Wolff and Barlow's (2000) "schizoid" children fit the Asperger syndrome phenotype and demonstrated the peaks and troughs seen in children with autism. On the other hand, Pomeroy and Friedman (2000) found that children with Asperger syndrome demonstrated weaknesses mainly in visuospatial ability (cited in Ehlers et al., 1997). Klin, Volkmar, Sparrow, Cicchetti, and Rourke (2003) suggested that children with Asperger syndrome demonstrated a cluster of neuropsychological assets and deficits seen in cognitive profiles of children with nonverbal learning disabilities. However, Bowler (1992) and Szatmari et al. (2000) did not find a significant difference between Verbal IQ and Performance IQ in children with Asperger syndrome. On the other hand, Ozonoff, Rogers, and Pennington (2000) reported that the large discrepancy found between the Verbal IQ and Performance IQ--in favor of the Verbal IQ--in 10 children diagnosed with Asperger syndrome was not found in the 13 children with high-functioning autism in their study. Children subtest scores were not reported in their research. Several theories have been proposed to explain the uneven cognitive performance among children with autism spectrum disorders and to serve as possible guideposts for assisting diagnosticians to discriminate among children with Asperger syndrome and those with high-functioning autism. Theory of mind deficits (Baron-Cohen, 1985), weak central coherence (Happe, 1994), and executive dysfunction (Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991) theories have received mixed results. Most research has found evidence of problems in all three of these areas among children with autism and evidence of executive dysfunction in children with Asperger syndrome (Ehlers et al., 1997). Ehlers et al. (1997) concluded that Asperger syndrome appears to share some neuropsychological dysfunction commonalities with autism, as well as deficits in attention, motor control, and perception. Conclusion Although it is difficult to identify children who are at the mild end of the autistic spectrum, however, it is not impossible. Every child is educationally and biologically unique and will need something slightly different. The challenge is to understand the range of differences that make up what are called the autistic spectrum disorders and then to understand the children differences that can be described in terms of slightly different profiles of strengths and weaknesses. The design of truly children treatment plans that exploit strengths and compensate for weaknesses begins with a detailed understanding of how learning is different for children with autism than for those without autism and how learning is different among different children with autism. References Asarnow, R. F., Tanguay, P. E., Bott, L., & Freeman, B. J. (2000). Patterns of intellectual functioning in non-retarded autistic and schizophrenic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 28, 273-280. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. Bowler, D. M. (1992). "Theory of mind" in Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33, 877-893. Dennis, M., Lockyet, L., Lazenby, A. L., Donnelly, R. E., Wilkinson, M., & Schoonheyt, W. (1999). Intelligence patterns among children with high-functioning autism, phenylketonuria, and childhood head injury. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 5-17. Ehlers, S., Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., Sandberg, A. D., Dahlgren, S., Hjelmquist, E., & Oden, A. (1997). Asperger syndrome, autism and attention deficit disorders: A comparative study of cognitive profiles of 120 children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 38, 207-217. Frith, U. (1991). Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge:CUP. Gerlach, Elizabeth K. 1996. Autism treatment guide. Oxford University Press; London Gernsbacher, M. A., Geye, H. M. & Ellis Weismer, S. (2005). The role of language and communication impairments within autism. In P. Fletcher & J. F. Miller (eds) Developmental Theory and Language Disorders. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (pp 73-94). Happe, F. (1994). Autism: an introduction to psychological theory. Sussex: Psychology Press. Hollander, E., Dolgoff-Kaspar, R, Cartwright, C., Rawitt, R, & Novotny, S. (2001). An open trial of divalproex sodium in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 62(7) , 530-534. Iovannone, R., Dunlap, G., Huber, H., & Kincaid, D. (2003). Effective educational practices for students with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on autism & other developmental disabilities, 18(3), 150-166. Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Cichetti, D. V., & Rourke, B. P. (2003). Validity and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger syndrome: Convergence with nonverbal learning disabilities syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36, 1127-1140. Le Couteur, H., McConachie, A. & Honey, E. (2005). Can a diagnosis of Asperger Syndrome be made in very young children with suspected Autistic Spectrum Disorder Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 167-176. Letts, C. & Leinonen, E. (2001). Comprehension of inferential meaning in language -impaired and language normal children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 36, 307-328. Lincoln, A. J., Courchesne, E., Kilman, B. A., Elmasian, R., & Allen, M. (2000). A study of intellectual abilities in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 505-524. Newport, Jerry, 2002. Autism-Asperger's & sexuality: puberty and beyond; Sage Publications, Bristol, UK. Norbury, C. F. & Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: a comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high functioning autism. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 37, 227-252 Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Asperger's syndrome: Evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1108-1122. Pomeroy, J., & Friedman, C.A. (2000). Asperger's syndrome: A clinical subtype of pervasive developmental disorders. Paper presented at the 36th annual meeting of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Los Angeles, CA. Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (2003). Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 155-168. Sattler, J. M. (1999). Assessment of children (3rd ed.). San Diego, CA: Author. Smith, P. K., Cowie, H. & Blades, M. (1998). Understanding Children's Development. (3rd Edition). Oxford: Blackwell. (Latest edition, 2003.) Spearman, C. E. (2001). The abilities of man. New York: Macmillan. Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, M. A. J., & Bartolucci, G. (2000). Asperger's syndrome and autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 130-136. Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-Third edition. New York: Psychological Corp. Wolff, S., & Barlow, A. (2000). Schizoid personality in childhood: A comparative study of schizoid, autistic, and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 20, 29-46. Read More
Cite this document
  • APA
  • MLA
  • CHICAGO
(“Autistic Spectrum Essay Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 1750 words”, n.d.)
Autistic Spectrum Essay Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 1750 words. Retrieved from https://studentshare.org/health-sciences-medicine/1523913-autistic-spectrum
(Autistic Spectrum Essay Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 1750 Words)
Autistic Spectrum Essay Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 1750 Words. https://studentshare.org/health-sciences-medicine/1523913-autistic-spectrum.
“Autistic Spectrum Essay Example | Topics and Well Written Essays - 1750 Words”, n.d. https://studentshare.org/health-sciences-medicine/1523913-autistic-spectrum.
  • Cited: 0 times

CHECK THESE SAMPLES OF Why Is It Difficult to Identify Children Who Are at the Mild End of the Autistic Spectrum

Asperger's Syndrome

Multifactorial factors and a shortage of research due to the relative newness of the disorder make it difficult to identify one specific cause for Asperger Syndrome.... This term paper focuses on recognition of Asperger syndrome as a specific disorder and responsibilities on those who work with and treat those individuals with the disorder to more fully understand and meet their special needs.... This term paper explores the topic of Asperger's Syndrome and describes not only it's terminology and common symptoms, but also focuses on educational intervention and support for children and adults diagnosed with it....
6 Pages (1500 words) Term Paper

Is Response Intervention effective in Autistic Children

Response to Intervention in autistic Children [Author's Name] [University] Abstract Is Response-to-Intervention (RTI) effective in autistic children?... This paper reflects the results of the study that involved autistic children and relied on RTI principles and constructs.... Keywords: autism, autistic, Response-to-Intervention, RTI.... Response to Intervention in autistic Children Response-to-Intervention is a popular object of present day research....
9 Pages (2250 words) Case Study

Cochlear Implant in Autistic Children

Even in the absence of a more comprehensive study, it is clear that cochlear implants can hugely benefit children with an autistic spectrum disorder.... It is difficult to implant the device in younger children due to difficulty in properly testing the hearing in such young patients.... The paper "Cochlear Implant in autistic Children" states that cochlear implant in deaf and autistic children not only helps them improve their auditory perception but also helps reduce the severity of their autism....
6 Pages (1500 words) Coursework

Autism and Autism Spectrum Disorder

This paper 'Autism and Autism spectrum Disorder' deals with Autism and Autism spectrum Disorder (ASD) and discusses wide-ranging teaching methods as interventionist approaches in classroom settings.... Autism is also known as an autism spectrum disorder, pervasive development disorder (PDD), PDD- not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS), atypical autism, and Asperger's Syndrome.... Autism spectrum Disorders (ASD) occurring early in life will last for the rest of one's life....
11 Pages (2750 words) Assignment

Autism Spectrum Disorder Experiment

(Edelson), agression, abnormal speech, and Because of their disruptive behaviours and communication disorders, children with ASD find it difficult to interact with other people (Hodgetts, Savage and McConnell).... This makes it difficult for healthcare professionals to.... Autism spectrum Disorder (ASD) is known for having impairment in social, cognitive, language and communication development (Hollander 3; MacKenzie 19).... ovaas conducted a long-term experiment which lasted for more than 2 years by assigning 19 autistic children to receive intensive treatment of more than 40 hours one-on-one intervention each week (experimental group) and another 19 autistic children to receive minimal treatment of a maximum of 10 hours one-on-one intervention each week (controlled group) (Lovaas a)....
4 Pages (1000 words) Essay

The Bizarre Actions of Different People

It may be that the autistic brain is not defective but simply different—an example of the variety of human development' (Wolman, 2008).... children who have autism face problems in all three core areas viz.... The paper "The Bizarre Actions of Different People" describes that ICT applications have enabled the autistics to metaphorically 'speak their minds', and in doing so have enabled them to reach across to the other spectrum of the population with the message: We're here....
11 Pages (2750 words) Case Study

Autism: The Needs of Children and Families

It must be noted here that severe autism is part of a spectrum, more commonly known as the Autism spectrum Disorder (ASD), which encompasses 'a range of ability levels and severities, but characterized by qualitative impairments in social, communicative and imaginative development' (Medical Research Council 2001, p.... One reason might be because those who suffer from autism do not have physical or facial deformities that are easily recognizable, in most cases, autistic children even possess an uncanny beauty that makes them seem all the more normal....
14 Pages (3500 words) Research Paper

Therapeutic Occupation and Intervention for a Group of Clients

he home environment of the autistic child is essentially characterized by the same elements as found in any other ordinary environment through the extent to which the home environment of the autistic child impacts on their behavior cannot be determined without a proper focus of attention on the environment related variables such as parental involvement in the dyslexic's behavior alteration efforts and attitudes of neighbors'....
14 Pages (3500 words) Research Proposal
sponsored ads
We use cookies to create the best experience for you. Keep on browsing if you are OK with that, or find out how to manage cookies.
Contact Us